Research Reports

The Associations of Peer-Rated Popularity and Likeability With Dark Triad Personality Traits in Adolescent Groups

Zsolt Péter Szabó*1,2§, Natália Zsuzsanna Orosz2§, Réka Gulyás2, András Láng3

Europe's Journal of Psychology, 2024, Vol. 20(3), 165–177, https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.11667

Received: 2023-03-29. Accepted: 2023-11-26. Published (VoR): 2024-08-30.

Handling Editor: Sahir Hussain, Victoria University of Wellington, Wellington, New Zealand

*Corresponding author at: Fővám tér 8., 1093 Budapest, Hungary. E-mail: zsoltpeter.szabo@uni-corvinus.hu

This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Abstract

One of the most significant challenges in adolescence is the pursuit of social acceptance, which can manifest in various forms, including likeability and popularity. Achieving social acceptance is associated with positive outcomes, while its absence is linked to adverse consequences. Existing research into the personality determinants impacting the ability to elicit likeability or gain popularity remains limited, primarily focusing on the influence of Big Five traits. This study aimed to explore the relationships between self-reported Dark Triad traits -encompassing Machiavellianism, subclinical psychopathy, and subclinical narcissism- and peer-rated likeability and popularity in a naturalistic high school classroom setting. The sample comprised 184 secondary students (98 females, 86 males) with an average age of 16.29 (SD = 1.36). Participants self-reported their Dark Triad traits and provided peer ratings through sociometric questions related to likeability and popularity. Our findings indicated that narcissism was significantly and positively associated with both likeability and popularity. In contrast, psychopathy and Machiavellianism exhibited minimal associations with measures of social acceptance. We discuss the theoretical and practical implications of these findings.

Keywords: Dark Triad personality traits, Machiavellianism, narcissism, psychopathy, social acceptance, likeability, popularity

One of the prominent challenges during adolescence is the pursuit of social acceptance, which can take on different forms, including likeability and popularity. Achieving social acceptance is linked to positive outcomes, such as improved mental health, happiness, and self-esteem (e.g., De Bruyn & Van Den Boom, 2005; Harris & Vazire, 2016). Conversely, its absence is associated with negative outcomes like loneliness (Zhang et al., 2014) and may even impact academic performance (Guinouard & Rychlak, 1962).

Understanding the determinants of social acceptance, especially during adolescence, is essential for promoting overall well-being and psychological development. The significance of personality traits was recognized early within the literature (Guinouard & Rychlak, 1962). The notion that dispositions such as selfishness, low empathy, limited agreeableness, and reduced honesty-humility, which are associated with Dark Triad personality traits (henceforth DT), might hold predictive power seems intuitively plausible. However, the existing body of literature addressing this issue is notably constrained in scope.

The investigation into the relationship between DT and social acceptance primarily focuses on their correlation with mate choice and romantic relationships (Jonason & Schmitt, 2012). This study aims to determine whether an association exists between elevated levels of DT among secondary school students and the likelihood of gaining popularity and/or likeability within the classroom.

Literature Background: Likeability and Popularity in Adolescent Groups: Relationships With Personality Traits

De Vries et al. (2020) argue that fulfilling the need to belong (i.e., social acceptance) presents a significant adaptive developmental challenge, which is tackled through two pathways: likeability and popularity. Likeability is defined as the extent to which peers like and accept an individual (Hubers et al., 2016), and it correlates with prosocial behavior and low aggression (van der Linden et al., 2010). Popularity refers to the extent to which an individual is viewed as socially dominant and visible within a group (Hubers et al., 2016). Popularity signifies the level of prestige and influence an individual holds within the group. van der Linden et al. (2010) assert that popularity is a multifaceted construct, encompassing favorable attributes such as intelligence, friendliness, and attractiveness, as well as less favorable traits like aggression, arrogance, and manipulativeness.

The concept of social acceptance as a finite resource, wherein likeability and popularity are viewed as scarce commodities (de Vries et al., 2020), has evolutionary roots. Acquiring peer relationships through likeability or popularity served as a valuable asset in evolutionary contexts (de Vries et al., 2020). It enabled individuals to establish bonds with peers to build a support network against threats. Furthermore, it facilitated the formation of relationships with the opposite sex for procreation.

The adaptive challenges rooted in evolutionary history no longer exert the same influence within the contemporary milieu as in their original context. However, the increased need for peer belongingness remains evident during adolescence (de Vries et al., 2020). In late adolescence, the potential pathways for satisfying these needs diverge: individuals who are popular begin to exhibit different behavior from those who are liked (Cillessen & Borch, 2006). While a strong correlation exists between likeability and popularity during early adolescence, this lessens in late adolescence. Being liked becomes less crucial for attaining popularity, which becomes more closely associated with the ability to ‘get ahead’ (de Vries et al., 2020). The prevailing consensus in the literature is that, despite their commonalities, likeability and popularity are distinct concepts (Wolters et al., 2014).

Existing research, relatively limited in scope, primarily focuses on the impact of the Big Five personality traits in exploring the personality determinants influence individuals’ ability to elicit likeability or gain popularity (Harris & Vazire, 2016).

Regarding likeability, extraversion, agreeableness, and emotional stability have emerged as dispositional determinants (de Vries et al., 2020; Harris & Vazire, 2016; Hubers et al., 2016; van der Linden et al., 2010). However, the role of extraversion remains somewhat ambiguous. While communal aspects of extraversion, like sociability, display a clear link with likeability, the relationship is less evident for agentic aspects of extraversion, such as dominance (Harris & Vazire, 2016).

The most consistent predictor of popularity is extraversion, while agreeableness does not significantly predict it, further hinting at the conceptual differences between popularity and likeability (Anderson et al., 2001; de Vries et al., 2020; Hubers et al., 2016; van der Linden et al., 2010).

De Vries et al. (2020) examined the relationships of the HEXACO personality model with likeability and popularity. In contrast to the Big Five, HEXACO incorporates a sixth dimension, honesty-humility, characterized by traits such as sincerity, fairness, and modesty. Their study revealed a notable correlation in early adolescence—an inverse link between honesty-humility and popularity. This suggests that individuals with higher levels of honesty-humility are less likely to achieve popularity, reinforcing the notion that popularity has a dark, antisocial aspect (LaFontana & Cillessen, 2002; Sandstrom & Cillessen, 2006; Wolters et al., 2014). These findings, along with similar observations (Cheng et al., 2010; Rauthmann & Kolar, 2013; Zeigler-Hill et al., 2014) emphasize the value of comprehensively exploring the interplay between popularity, likeability, and DT to enhance our understanding of social acceptance in adolescent groups.

The Dark Triad and Social Acceptance

The DT comprises three socially undesirable personality traits: Machiavellianism, subclinical narcissism, and subclinical psychopathy (Paulhus & Williams, 2002). These traits share a common core characterized by malevolent and exploitative behavior, low willingness to cooperate, low honesty-humility, and a propensity towards callousness and aggression (Kowalski et al., 2021). Despite these commonalities, significant differences exist among the three traits. These differences may be closely linked to variations in how individuals high in each trait are perceived by their peers.

Subclinical narcissism, particularly the agentic and grandiose types measured by the Short Dark Triad (Jones & Paulhus, 2014), is defined by grandiosity, entitlement, superiority, and dominance (Paulhus & Williams, 2002). Individuals high in grandiose subclinical narcissism typically display reactive aggression in response to ego-threatening situations. Subclinical psychopathy is marked by indifference toward social norms, impulsivity, hostile and instrumental aggressive behaviors, risk-taking tendencies, and low empathy (Truhan et al., 2021). Unlike the other two traits, Machiavellianism is characterized by its long-term, strategic, and flexible adaptation to changes in the social environment, as well as low levels of aggression (Bereczkei, 2018).

A notable research gap exists in understanding how individuals with DT are perceived and accepted in close-knit groups. Moreover, even the limited existing studies typically rely on self-report data in non-naturalistic settings, potentially leading to invalid conclusions (Muris et al., 2017).

While the negative aspects of grandiose subclinical narcissism undoubtedly present challenges within most long-term social groups (Prusik & Szulawski, 2019), some argue that its positive traits, coupled with its associations with extraversion (Szabó, Czibor et al., 2023; Szabó, Diller et al., 2023), can make individuals with high grandiose narcissism likable and popular. Recent research establishes connections between narcissism and attributes such as mental toughness and resilience (Papageorgiou et al., 2019; Szabó et al., 2022), enhancing their potential for advancement and visibility. Studies demonstrate that narcissistic traits often contribute to success (Rauthmann & Kolar, 2013) and leadership roles (Paunonen et al., 2006).

Research focusing on narcissism’s impact on forming and maintaining friendships, an aspect tightly linked to likeability (Laursen et al., 2023), reveals that individuals with elevated narcissism levels establish friendships for various reasons, including factors that establish the foundation for enduring relationships, like shared interests (Jonason & Schmitt, 2012). However, those with narcissistic traits exhibit reduced friendship commitment (Sauls & Zeigler-Hill, 2020), and narcissism is also linked to challenges in forging and maintaining relationships (Wehner & Ziegler, 2023). In naturalistic settings, their evaluations by others tend to be neutral (Rauthmann, 2012). Reactive aggression, also tied to narcissism, negatively predicts popularity (Stoltz et al., 2016).

High psychopathy scores are associated with confrontational behavior, hostile and instrumental aggression, and bullying (Boddy, 2010). The ability of individuals with high psychopathy scores to attain high status and popularity in adult groups remains a subject of considerable debate. While a substantial body of research suggests that they can advance through aggressive, risk-taking, and manipulative behaviors (e.g., Babiak et al., 2010), recent findings challenge this view. A meta-analysis discovered no discernible link between psychopathy scores and leadership emergence and effectiveness (Landay et al., 2019). The intricacy intensifies within adolescent groups, where the factors that typically hinder individuals with high psychopathy scores in work settings, such as impulsivity, deviant behavior, and resistance to authority, might paradoxically construct an appealing antihero image (Jonason et al., 2012). Supporting this notion, Davis et al. (2022) identified a positive association between psychopathy and dating success among adolescents.

It is unlikely that individuals with psychopathic traits are particularly liked by their peers as they are probably not seen as friendly and cooperative (van der Linden et al., 2010). Self-reports indicate they form friendships with volatile others (Jonason & Schmitt, 2012), and employ distant or indirect communication when ending a friendship (Brewer et al., 2023). In the perceptions of others, they are predominantly viewed negatively (Rauthmann, 2012).

Forecasting the popularity and likeability of individuals possessing high levels of Machiavellianism presents a challenge due to the flexible and adaptable nature of their behavior. Previous research reveals a connection between Machiavellianism and a spectrum of both anti- and prosocial behaviors (e.g., Szabó et al., 2018). Anchored in the pursuit of personal success through diverse means, their behavior remains shaped by strategic objectives. For instance, Becker and O’Hair (2007) suggest that individuals with high levels of Machiavellianism exhibit altruistic behaviors when such actions aid in manipulating others’ perceptions of them. However, they tend to choose friends who are more vulnerable to exploitation (Jonason & Schmitt, 2012). In their friendships, they exhibit insincerity, demonstrate restricted self-disclosure, and may even display happiness in response to their friends’ misfortunes (Abell & Brewer, 2018; Brewer et al., 2014). They are largely seen negatively by others (Rauthmann, 2012).

Rogoza et al. (2021) investigated the simultaneous impact of the Dark Triad traits on relationship formation within a longitudinal and naturalistic setting. They collected self-report data on DT and obtained peer ratings from high-school classmates. Peers were asked to identify the individuals they liked the most in the classroom and those they perceived as leaders. The first question directly measured likeability, while the second question indirectly assessed popularity, or as argued by the authors, peer-rated agency. The study yielded complex relationships among the variables.

In the short term, psychopathy was negatively related to likeability, while Machiavellianism showed a positive relationship, and narcissism exhibited no significant association. Conversely, in the long term, only narcissism displayed a connection with likeability, acting as a negative predictor. This indicates that individuals with narcissistic traits were less favored by their peers over an extended time, in line with similar findings by Paulhus (1998).

In the short term, psychopathy was negatively related to leadership selection, while Machiavellianism showed a positive relationship, and narcissism exhibited no significant association. However, in the long term, narcissism emerged as the sole significant predictor of leadership selections. Specifically, individuals with narcissistic traits displayed a higher likelihood of being chosen as leaders, highlighting a peer-reported agency associated with narcissism. Closing this section, we would like to discuss the evolutionary psychological approach to DT. As mentioned earlier, some literature (de Vries et al., 2020) views the achievement of likeability and popularity as a solution to the evolutionary problem of belongingness. The evolutionary approach to DT has conceptualized these traits as condition-dependent adaptations to address life’s challenges within an unpredictable environment (Jonason et al., 2016). While directly testing this idea is almost impossible, it theoretically makes sense to consider DT as solutions for fulfilling belongingness needs through the attainment of social dominance, i.e., popularity.

Specifically, according to life history theory, individuals develop a life history shaped by their early experiences, which can be positioned along the slow-fast continuum (Belsky et al., 1991; Del Giudice et al., 2015). A fast life history can be characterized by an untrustful internal working model, utilitarian interpersonal relationships, early puberty, and sexual activity, among others. There is a close link between DT and a fast life history (Birkás et al., 2020; however, for an in-depth discussion about the complex relationships between DT and life history see Manson, 2020; McDonald et al., 2012). Many attributes of DT, such as impulsivity and entitlement, can be perceived as adaptive strategies developed in response to harsh and unpredictable environments (Jonason et al., 2016; McDonald et al., 2012).

Living a fast life is probably associated with the tendency to seek popularity—that is, dominance, visibility, and prestige. Essentially, a fast life history aligns with a ‘getting ahead’ motivation rather than a ‘getting along’ motivation. Adolescents in their turbulent and stressful development stage might take advantage of DT related to fast life history to achieve, often short-lived, popularity in peer groups which would be reflected in their higher popularity scores.

However, we do not posit a complete correspondence between DT and popularity. Specific DT attributes, such as aggression, can simultaneously promote and hinder popularity attainment, contingent on the type of aggression—proactive or reactive (Stoltz et al., 2016)

The Present Study

The present study surveyed secondary school classes to explore the associations of self-reported DT with peer-rated likeability and popularity. The latter was measured through both direct and indirect means (peer-rated agency). Considering the conflicting possibilities and inconsistent findings in the limited existing research that has examined these matters, we are posing research questions. Nevertheless, some expectations can still be articulated, such as the anticipation that DT will exhibit a closer connection to popularity than to likeability.

Questions related to likeability (RQ1): What is the correlation between students’ self-reported DT and their peer-rated likeability? Do students with high self-reported Dark Triad traits receive favorable opinions and acceptance, and are they perceived as friendly and cooperative?

Questions related to popularity (RQ2): What is the correlation between students’ self-reported DT and their peer-rated popularity? Are students with high self-reported DT traits seen as popular among their peers and teachers? Additionally, are students with high-self reported DT seen as potential leaders among their peers and perceived to have successfully adapted to school life and possess great potential for success in life?

Importantly, the present study observed the associations between DT, likeability, and popularity in a highly naturalistic setting and with the employment of peer ratings. The literature reviewed above reveals that most of the relevant studies were conducted in less naturalistic settings and/or were based on self-report data.

The data, materials, and analysis syntax of the study are openly available at Szabó (2024).

Method

Sample

The study involved 184 secondary students (98 females and 86 males; MAGE = 16.29, SD = 1.36) from nine classes enrolled in four-year secondary programs. Class size varied between 15 and 35 students. A post-hoc power analysis using G*Power (Faul et al., 2009) yielded acceptable power numbers (ranging between 0.52 and 0.99).

Design and Procedure

Data were collected between September 2020 and January 2021 during the COVID-19 pandemic. Copies of the test battery were distributed among the students by their respective class teachers, as researchers were not authorized to enter the school site due to epidemiological reasons. To minimize the occurrence of missing cases, participants who were absent from school during their respective survey sessions were granted online access to the survey. Survey completion took approximately 45 minutes.

Before participating, each participant was informed about the study’s purpose and the relevant data protection provisions, particularly concerning the confidential handling of their data. Data collectors also ensured that all participants were at least 14 years of age. Following the provision of informed consent and demographic data, participants completed a self-report measure of the Dark Triad traits, followed by a sociometric inventory. Each participant submitted their response in a sealed envelope. Although collected data included participants’ names, we assured participants that their personal information would not be shared with third parties, including teachers, classmates, and parents.

Additionally, parental consent and institutional approval from the heads of the participating high schools were obtained before data collection. The study was conducted with ethical approval from Eötvös Loránd University.

Measures

Dark Triad Personality Traits

We assessed DT using the 27-item Short Dark Triad (SD3; Jones & Paulhus, 2014), which was adapted to Hungarian by Szabó, Czibor et al. (2023). Each trait was measured by nine items: Machiavellianism (e.g., “It’s not wise to tell your secrets”; Cronbach’s α = 0.65), subclinical narcissism (e.g., “People see me as a natural leader”; Cronbach’s α = 0.76), and subclinical psychopathy (e.g., “People who mess with me always regret it”; Cronbach’s α = 0.71). Participants rated items on a 5-point scale ranging from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree.” Composite scores were computed by calculating the mean of the corresponding items.

Likeability

Likeability was measured with six items focusing on intimate interpersonal relationships within the class: ‘Who is your best friend in the class?’; ‘Who would you first ask for help if you were in trouble?’; ‘With whom would you first share some interesting news?’; ‘Whom do you intend to stay in touch with after graduation?’; ‘Whom do you like the most in your class?’; ‘With whom would you first share a personal secret?’. Responses to these items were combined into a single composite variable labeled Likeability, which represented the sum of votes received by each participant across the six items.

Popularity

The measurement of popularity utilized two distinct methods. For the direct measure of popularity, participants were asked to identify the most popular members within their groups. We used two items to assess perceived popularity among peers (‘Who are the most popular among students in your class?’) and teachers (‘Who are the most popular students among teachers?’). This distinction enabled an exploration of potential variations in factors influencing popularity among students, as opposed to perceived popularity among authority figures. Importantly, it is crucial to clarify that both the perceived popularity among peers and teachers were evaluated through responses from peers. Teachers were not surveyed in this study.

Indirect measures of popularity assessed different aspects of agency, each captured by a single item: Leadership ability (‘Which of your classmates would be the best leader?’), Organizing skills (‘If your class teacher was unable to manage the class for an extended period, which of your classmates do you believe would be the most suitable replacement?’), Future success (‘Among your classmates, who do you believe will achieve the most success in life?’), and Adjustment to school life (‘Among your classmates, who appears most comfortable with school life?’).

Participants could select as many classmates as they wished for each question. To ensure data consistency across classes of varying sizes for each sociometric measure, the total number of votes received by each participant on each measure was divided by the total number of students in the participant’s class. Additional sociometric data on attractiveness and giftedness were collected but were not analyzed in the present data.

Results

First, we investigated the correlations between DT and the sociometric variables (see Table 1).

Table 1

Spearman Correlations and Descriptive Statistics for DT Traits

VariableMachiavellianismNarcissismPsychopathy
Likeability.17*.17*.12†
Perceived popularity among peers.17*.33**.16*
Perceived popularity among teachers-.01-.05-.16*
Leadership ability.12.31**.13†
Organizing skills.06.13.03
Future success.13†.28**.08
Adjustment to school life-.07.08-.01
M3.612.712.31
SD0.600.710.68

Note. Correlation coefficients are Spearman’s rho.

p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01.

The Dark Triad traits exhibited weak, but statistically significant positive correlations with likeability. Narcissism displayed positive correlations with the number of votes related to perceived popularity among peers, perceived popularity among teachers, leadership, and potential for success in life. Machiavellianism was positively linked with perceived popularity among peers and exhibited a marginally significant correlation with perceived potential for success in life. Psychopathy displayed a positive correlation with perceived popularity among classmates while showing a negative correlation with perceived popularity among teachers.

Due to the relatively high frequency of zero values in each sociometric measure (ranging from 41.3% to 69%), a two-step analysis procedure, as proposed by Boulton and Williford (2018), was employed. For each sociometric measure, except likeability which possessed a continuous nature, a two-variable approach was adopted. This approach involved creating a binary variable (0 = no vote, 1 = one or more votes) and a continuous variable. In the continuous variable, zero values were excluded from subsequent analysis as missing data.

The binary variables were entered as outcome variables in uniform binary logistic regression models, using the DT as predictors and gender as a control variable. The continuous variables were included in linear regression models using the same predictor and control variables as the binary logistic models. We applied a Bonferroni-corrected p-value of .008 to determine the significance in the binary logistic regressions and .007 in the linear regressions. This correction involved dividing the threshold of .05 by the number of multiple comparisons.

In the binary logistic regressions, we report standardized beta weights (β), Wald statistics, Cox and Snell R2, and Nagelkerke R2. The results, presented in Table 2, indicated that narcissism was a significant predictor of perceived popularity among peers, perceived leadership ability, and perceived likelihood of future success. However, Machiavellianism and psychopathy did not emerge as significant predictors of the outcome variables.

Table 2

Results of Binary Logistic Regression on Popularity

DV1: Perceived popularity among peers
DV2: Perceived popularity among teachers
DV3: Leadership ability
VariableβSEWaldpβSEWaldpβSEWaldp
Mach.39.291.80.18.02.27.01.94.17.30.32.58
Narc.97.2713.26<.001-.03.23.01.91.80.269.13.003
Psych.07.27.06.80-.20.25.61.44.24.27.77.38
Gender-.72.334.70.03-.51.312.71.10-.02.34.01.94
Cox and Snell R2.12.02.08
Nagelkerke R2.16.03.12
DV4: Organizing skills
DV5: Future success
DV6: Adjustment to school life
VariableβSEWaldpβSEWaldpβSEWaldp
Mach.23.28.64.43.16.28.34.56-.52.293.10.08
Narc.19.24.64.43.64.256.55.01.23.25.85.36
Psych.07.26.08.77-.02.26.00.95.13.27.23.63
Gender-.28.32.77.38-.25.32.62.43.52.332.52.11
Cox and Snell R2.01.05.04
Nagelkerke R2.02.07.05

Note. Mach = Machiavellianism. Narc = Subclinical narcissism. Psych = Subclinical psychopathy.

Table 3 presents the results of the linear regression analyses. Narcissism emerged as a significant positive predictor of perceived leadership ability and perceived organizational skills. Individuals exhibiting narcissistic traits were also more likely to be perceived as destined for future success. Additionally, several other associations reached the conventional significance threshold (p < .05) but did not achieve Bonferroni-corrected significance (p < .07). For instance, narcissism was a positive predictor of perceived popularity among peers (p = .05) and perceived popularity among teachers (p = .01), while psychopathy was a negative predictor of perceived popularity among teachers (p = .02). Psychopathy was also a negative predictor of perceived leadership abilities (p = .02) and the perceived ability to adapt to school life (p = .04). Machiavellianism was a positive predictor of the perceived ability to fit into school life (p = .02).

Table 3

Regression Analyses on Likeability and Popularity

DV1: Perceived popularity among peers
DV2: Perceived popularity among teachers
VariableβSEβ 95% CIpβSEβ 95% CIp
Mach.00.02-.04, .05.91.04.03-.01, .09.12
Narc.04.02.00, .08.05.06.02.02, .10.01
Psych.01.02-.04, .05.76-.06.02-.10, -.01.02
Gender-.04.03-.02, .09.19-.03.03-.03, .09.29
Model summary: R2 = .06, F(4, 95) = 1.48, p = .22Model summary R2 = .13, F(4, 81) = 3.11, p = .02
DV3: Leadership abilities
DV4: Organizing skills
VariableβSEβ 95% CIpβSEβ 95% CIp
Mach.03.03-.03, .09.38-.02.03-.08, .04.42
Narc.09.03.04, .14.001.10.03.05, .15<.001
Psych-.06.02-.11, -.01.02-.05.03-.10, .01.10
Gender-.09.03.03, .15.01-.06.03.00, .13.05
Model summary: R2 = .30, F(4, 52) = 5.43, p = .001Model summary R2 = .24, F(4, 59) = 4.61, p = .003
DV5: Future success
DV6: Adjustment to school life
VariableβSEβ 95% CIpβSEβ 95% CIp
Mach.00.02-.04, .03.90.05.02.01, .09.02
Narc.05.02.02, .08.001.01.02-.03, .04.69
Psych-.01.02-.04, .02.53-.04.02-.07, .00.04
Gender-.06.02.02, .10.01-.03.02-.01, .07.15
Model summary: R2 = .13, F(4, 103) = 3.94, p = .005Model summary R2 = .13, F(4, 56) = 2.05, p = .10
DV7: Likeability
VariableβSEβ 95% CIp
Mach.01.01.00, .03.10
Narc.01.01.00, .03.05
Psych.01.01.00, .03.14
Gender-.03.01.02, .05< .001
Model summary: R2 = .12, F(4, 179) = 6.29, p < .001

Note. Mach = Machiavellianism. Narc = Subclinical narcissism. Psych = Subclinical psychopathy.

Discussion

The present study investigated the associations between DT, likeability, and popularity in a naturalistic setting, employing both self-rated and other-rated data.

The most consistent results were obtained for narcissism. Specifically, individuals with higher self-reported narcissism were perceived by their classmates as popular and likable. They were also seen as possessing strong leadership and organizational abilities. Furthermore, they were more frequently identified as having substantial potential for post-secondary school success compared to individuals with lower narcissistic traits. The predictive impact of psychopathy and Machiavellianism in terms of likeability and popularity was found to be minimal. Some indications emerged that individuals with high psychopathy and Machiavellianism scores are marginally more liked by their peers and slightly more popular compared to other students. Furthermore, individuals with elevated Machiavellianism scores exhibit adeptness at effectively adapting to their current social environment. However, when considering the broader context, our findings surprisingly suggest that neither Machiavellianism nor psychopathy is correlated with social acceptance within adolescent groups.

Due to the limitations of our study design, our results are primarily observational, and the explanations we provide are inherently speculative. Nevertheless, we consider these observations valuable as they contribute to our understanding of two important questions in the literature: (1) the factors that account for the likeability and popularity of members within adolescent groups, and (2) the perception of individuals with Dark Triad traits by their closely associated peers.

The likeability and popularity associated with narcissism could potentially demonstrate the accuracy of self-perceptions among individuals with elevated narcissism. Future studies should investigate in greater detail whether individuals with high narcissism levels are indeed more likable and popular compared to those with lower narcissistic tendencies. Alternatively, these results are only illustrations of the transient effectiveness of narcissistic individuals’ self-perception strategies, rather than unequivocally indicating their genuine acceptance among peers. Rogoza et al. (2021) have demonstrated dynamic shifts in the likeability and perceived leadership abilities of individuals with elevated narcissism over the long term.

Nevertheless, it is intriguing to compare our findings with those of Rogoza et al. (2021). In their study, they initially found no significant relationship between narcissism and likeability or perceived leadership ability. However, in the long term, narcissism exhibited a negative correlation with likeability and a positive correlation with perceived leadership ability. This suggests that individuals with higher narcissism scores were seemingly regarded by their peers as more capable leaders in both studies. Their ‘born leader’ attitude could either be grounded in realistic self-assessments or function as a self-fulfilling prophecy. However, the extent to which peers can form positive likeability perceptions of individuals with elevated narcissism scores remains an open question.

Rogoza et al. (2021), akin to our study, employed the SD3 to assess Dark Triad traits. This assessment predominantly captures the agentic aspects of narcissism. Future studies might consider utilizing measures that encompass additional aspects of narcissism such as vulnerability and antagonism (Crowe et al., 2019).

The null effects associated with Machiavellianism and psychopathy may be attributed to the simultaneous influence of Machiavellianism and narcissism characteristics, which contribute to both promoting popularity and eliciting rejection. The inclination of individuals with high psychopathy scores to rebel against authority, take risks, and engage in instrumental aggression may facilitate their attainment of social dominance. However, simultaneously, their harassment of peers, hostile aggression, and self-centered, exploitative behaviors may easily overshadow any popularity they may have gained.

Likewise, the adaptive behavioral strategies characteristic of Machiavellians could function both in favor of and against the attainment of popularity. Individuals with high Machiavellianism scores tend to favor partners they can exploit (Jonason & Schmitt, 2012) and they display self-serving behaviors even within collaborative groups (Bereczkei et al., 2015). This may lead to greater success in short-term relationships, yet it can also lead to conflicting evaluations within communities where members interact daily over an extended period, such as a high school classroom. While speculative, our analysis leads us to conclude that the advantageous and disadvantageous facets of psychopathy and Machiavellianism, in terms of fostering popularity, may counterbalance each other, resulting in null effects.

Based on these findings, it would certainly be a far-fetched conclusion to suggest that Machiavellianism and psychopathy are evolutionary solutions for the adaptive developmental problem of peer belongingness needs (de Vries et al., 2020). On the other hand, narcissism was positively associated with both likeability and popularity. It seems that narcissistic traits such as self-confidence, grandiosity, exhibitionism, and a ‘born leader’ attitude may appeal to members of an adolescent peer group (Jonason et al., 2012). Thus, the obtained findings for narcissism are consistent with previously proposed evolutionary conceptualizations (e.g., Jonason et al., 2016).

Limitations and Future Studies

Several limitations should be acknowledged. The cross-sectional, non-longitudinal design of the study precludes analyzing the DT’s associations with long-term social success. Future research could adopt a longitudinal design to explore temporal changes in the relationship between DT and the attainment of likeability and popularity.

Another limitation is the relatively small sample size due to the then-active coronavirus pandemic. This implies that any observed effects should be interpreted with caution. However, we believe that the richness of our data compensates for this limitation. For instance, we utilized 12 sociometric items to access likeability and popularity, while Rogoza et al. (2021), in a comparable sociometric investigation, only employed two items. Our sample size is comparable to those used in previous studies investigating the likeability and popularity of individuals with high DT (see e.g., Jauk et al., 2016; Rauthmann, 2012; Rogoza et al., 2021).

Additional limitations pertain to the instruments employed in this study. The SD3, though practical, primarily focuses on the agentic aspect of narcissism, neglecting facets like antagonism and vulnerability that could impact social acceptance. Also, the Machiavellianism subscale’s Cronbach’s alpha fell below the traditional cutoff (Nunnally, 1978). Future studies should employ more reliable, age-specific measures and expand assessment scope by evaluating each DT trait separately using comprehensive measures. Future research could also explore additional dark traits, such as everyday sadism (Buckels et al., 2013).

Conclusion

The primary outcome of our research indicates a clear correlation between narcissism and both likeability and popularity. Psychopathy and Machiavellianism, on the other hand, exhibit minimal associations with factors measuring social acceptance.

Understanding the determinants of peer acceptance during adolescence holds both academic intrigue and practical significance. The existing literature suggests that individuals who do not achieve either pathway of peer acceptance -meaning they are neither popular nor liked- experience negative consequences in the short and long term, impacting their well-being and potentially their academic performance. Consequently, any insights that deepen our understanding of peer acceptance during adolescence can equip educators with valuable tools to support a broader range of students in gaining peer acceptance.

Our findings could also foster the development of empathy and perspective-taking among professionals working with young individuals. These findings show that personality traits conventionally labeled as dark, such as narcissism, can also be seen as solutions to adaptive developmental challenges. Despite the evident drawbacks of inflated self-esteem, entitlement, and exploitation of others, these behaviors can also be interpreted as adolescents’ attempts to establish dominance, prestige, visibility, and, consequently, a sense of belongingness.

Funding

Zsolt Péter Szabó declares that Project No. TKP2021-NKTA-19 has been implemented with the support provided from the National Research, Development, Innovation Fund of Hungary, and financed under the TKP2021-NKTA funding scheme. András Láng was funded by the National Research, Development and Innovation Office (Grant No. NRDI-138040).

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the schools, the teachers, and all student participants for their collaboration with the research on which this paper is based.

Competing Interests

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Ethics Statement

All procedures in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional research committee and with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association, the 1964 Helsinki declaration, and its later amendments. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study. Additionally, parental consent and institutional approval from the heads of the participating high schools were obtained before data collection.

Data Availability

The data, materials, and analysis syntax of the study are openly available at Szabó (2024).

References

  • Abell, L., & Brewer, G. (2018). Machiavellianism and schadenfreude in women’s friendships. Psychological Reports, 121(5), 909-919. https://doi.org/10.1177/0033294117741652

  • Anderson, C., John, O. P., Keltner, D., & Kring, A. M. (2001). Who attains social status? Effects of personality and physical attractiveness in social groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(1), 116-132. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.1.116

  • Babiak, P., Neumann, C. S., & Hare, R. D. (2010). Corporate psychopathy: Talking the walk. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 28(2), 174-193. https://doi.org/10.1002/bsl.925

  • Becker, J. A. H., & O’Hair, H. D. (2007). Machiavellians’ motives in organizational citizenship behavior. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 35(3), 246-267. https://doi.org/10.1080/00909880701434232

  • Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., & Draper, P. (1991). Childhood experience, interpersonal development, and reproductive strategy: An evolutionary theory of socialization. Child Development, 62(4), 647-670. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131166

  • Bereczkei, T. (2018). Machiavellian intelligence hypothesis revisited: What evolved cognitive and social skills may underlie human manipulation. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 12(1), 32-51. https://doi.org/10.1037/ebs0000096

  • Bereczkei, T., Szabó, Z. P., & Czibor, A. (2015). Abusing good intentions: Machiavellians strive for exploiting cooperators. SAGE Open, 5(2), https://doi.org/10.1177/2158244015593119

  • Birkás, B., Pátkai, G., & Csathó, Á. (2020). The mediating role of the Dark Triad between life history strategy and perceived stress factors. Psychological Reports, 123(2), 252-265. https://doi.org/10.1177/0033294118818095

  • Boddy, C. R. P. (2010). Corporate psychopaths and organizational type. Journal of Public Affairs, 10(4), 300-312. https://doi.org/10.1002/pa.365

  • Boulton, A. J., & Williford, A. (2018). Analyzing skewed continuous outcomes with many zeros: A tutorial for social work and youth prevention science researchers. Journal of the Society for Social Work and Research, 9(4), 721-740. https://doi.org/10.1086/701235

  • Brewer, G., Abell, L., & Lyons, M. (2014). Machiavellianism, competition and self-disclosure in friendship. Individual Differences Research, 12(1), 1-7.

  • Brewer, G., Parkinson, M., Pickles, A., Anson, J., & Mulinder, G. (2023). Dark Triad traits and relationship dissolution. Personality and Individual Differences, 204, Article 112045. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2022.112045

  • Buckels, E. E., Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2013). Behavioral confirmation of everyday sadism. Psychological Science, 24(11), 2201-2209. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613490749

  • Cheng, J. T., Tracy, J. L., & Henrich, J. (2010). Pride, personality, and the evolutionary foundations of human social status. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(5), 334-347. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.02.004

  • Cillessen, A. H. N., & Borch, C. (2006). Developmental trajectories of adolescent popularity: A growth curve modelling analysis. Journal of Adolescence, 29(6), 935-959. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2006.05.005

  • Crowe, M. L., Lynam, D. R., Campbell, W. K., & Miller, J. D. (2019). Exploring the structure of narcissism: Toward an integrated solution. Journal of Personality, 87(6), 1151-1169. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12464

  • Davis, A. C., Brittain, H., Arnocky, S., & Vaillancourt, T. (2022). Longitudinal associations between primary and secondary psychopathic traits, delinquency, and current dating status in adolescence. Evolutionary Psychology, 20(1), https://doi.org/10.1177/14747049211068670

  • De Bruyn, E. H., & Van Den Boom, D. C. (2005). Interpersonal behavior, peer popularity and self-esteem in early adolescence. Social Development, 14(4), 555-573. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2005.00317.x

  • Del Giudice, M., Gangestad, S. W., & Kaplan, H. S. (2015). Life history theory and evolutionary psychology. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), Handbook of evolutionary psychology (2nd ed., pp. 88–114). Wiley.

  • de Vries, R. E., Pronk, J., Olthof, T., & Goossens, F. A. (2020). Getting along and/or getting ahead: Differential HEXACO personality correlates of likeability and popularity among adolescents. European Journal of Personality, 34(2), 245-261. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2243

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149-1160. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149

  • Guinouard, D. E., & Rychlak, J. F. (1962). Personality correlates of sociometric popularity in elementary school children. Personnel and Guidance Journal, 40(5), 438-442. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2164-4918.1962.tb02136.x

  • Harris, K., & Vazire, S. (2016). On friendship development and the Big Five personality traits. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 10(11), 647-667. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12287

  • Hubers, M. D., Burk, W. J., Segers, E., Kleinjan, M., Scholte, R. H., & Cillessen, A. H. N. (2016). Personality and problem behaviours as predictors of adolescents’ social status: Academic track and gender as moderators. Educational Psychology, 36(8), 1443-1461. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2015.1053841

  • Jauk, E., Neubauer, A. C., Mairunteregger, T., Pemp, S., Sieber, K. P., & Rauthmann, J. F. (2016). How alluring are dark personalities? The Dark Triad and attractiveness in speed dating. European Journal of Personality, 30(2), 125-138. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2040

  • Jonason, P. K., Icho, A., & Ireland, K. (2016). Resources, harshness, and unpredictability: The socioeconomic conditions associated with the Dark Triad traits. Evolutionary Psychology, 14(1), https://doi.org/10.1177/1474704915623699

  • Jonason, P. K., & Schmitt, D. P. (2012). What have you done for me lately? Friendship-selection in the shadow of the Dark Triad traits. Evolutionary Psychology, 10(3), 400-421. https://doi.org/10.1177/147470491201000303

  • Jonason, P. K., Webster, G. D., Schmitt, D. P., Li, N. P., & Crysel, L. (2012). The antihero in popular culture: Life history theory and the Dark Triad personality traits. Review of General Psychology, 16(2), 192-199. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027914

  • Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2014). Introducing the Short Dark Triad (SD3): A brief measure of dark personality traits. Assessment, 21(1), 28-41. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191113514105

  • Kowalski, C. M., Rogoza, R., Saklofske, D. H., & Schermer, J. A. (2021). Dark triads, tetrads, tents, and cores: Why navigate (research) the jungle of dark personality models without a compass (criterion)? Acta Psychologica, 221, Article 103455. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2021.103455

  • LaFontana, K. M., & Cillessen, A. H. N. (2002). Children’s perceptions of popular and unpopular peers: A multimethod assessment. Developmental Psychology, 38(5), 635-647. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.38.5.635

  • Landay, K., Harms, P. D., & Credé, M. (2019). Shall we serve the dark lords? A meta-analytic review of psychopathy and leadership. Journal of Applied Psychology, 104(1), 183-196. https://doi.org/10.1037/apl0000357

  • Laursen, B., Leggett-James, M. P., & Valdes, O. M. (2023). Relative likeability and relative popularity as sources of influence in children’s friendships. PLoS One, 18(5), Article e0283117. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0283117

  • Manson, J. H. (2020). Is narcissism a slow life history strategy indicator? The answer depends on the LHS instrument. Evolutionary Psychology, 18(3), https://doi.org/10.1177/1474704920946236

  • McDonald, M. M., Donnellan, M. B., & Navarrete, C. D. (2012). A life history approach to understanding the Dark Triad. Personality and Individual Differences, 52(5), 601-605. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.12.003

  • Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., Otgaar, H., & Meijer, E. (2017). The malevolent side of human nature: A meta-analysis and critical review of the literature on the Dark Triad (narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy). Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(2), 183-204. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616666070

  • Nunnally, J. C. (1978). Psychometric theory (2nd ed.). McGraw-Hill.

  • Papageorgiou, K. A., Benini, E., Bilello, D., Gianniou, F. M., Clough, P. J., & Costantini, G. (2019). Bridging the gap: A network approach to Dark Triad, mental toughness, the Big Five, and perceived stress. Journal of Personality, 87(6), 1250-1263. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12472

  • Paulhus, D. L. (1998). Interpersonal and intrapsychic adaptiveness of trait self-enhancement: A mixed blessing? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5), 1197-1208. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1197

  • Paulhus, D. L., & Williams, K. M. (2002). The Dark Triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy. Journal of Research in Personality, 36(6), 556-563. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00505-6

  • Paunonen, S. V., Lönnqvist, J.-E., Verkasalo, M., Leikas, S., & Nissinen, V. (2006). Narcissism and emergent leadership in military cadets. Leadership Quarterly, 17(5), 475-486. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2006.06.003

  • Prusik, M., & Szulawski, M. (2019). The relationship between the Dark Triad personality traits, motivation at work, and burnout among HR recruitment workers. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, Article 1290. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01290

  • Rauthmann, J. F. (2012). The Dark Triad and interpersonal perception: Similarities and differences in the social consequences of narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy. Social Psychological & Personality Science, 3(4), 487-496. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550611427608

  • Rauthmann, J. F., & Kolar, G. P. (2013). Positioning the Dark Triad in the interpersonal circumplex: The friendly-dominant narcissist, hostile-submissive Machiavellian, and hostile-dominant psychopath? Personality and Individual Differences, 54(5), 622-627. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.11.021

  • Rogoza, R., Danieluk, B., Kowalski, C. M., Kwiatkowska, K., & Kwiatkowska, M. M. (2021). Making and maintaining relationships through the prism of the Dark Triad traits: A longitudinal social network study. Journal of Personality, 89(2), 338-356. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12585

  • Sandstrom, M. J., & Cillessen, A. H. N. (2006). Likeable versus popular: Distinct implications for adolescent adjustment. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30(4), 305-314. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025406072789

  • Sauls, D., & Zeigler-Hill, V. (2020). The narcissistic experience of friendship: The roles of agentic and communal orientations toward friendship. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 37(10–11), 2693-2713. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407520933685

  • Stoltz, S., Cillessen, A. H. N., van den Berg, Y. H., & Gommans, R. (2016). Popularity differentially predicts reactive and proactive aggression in early adolescence. Aggressive Behavior, 42(1), 29-40. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21603

  • Szabó, Z. P. (2024). Dark Triad likeability popularity [OSF project page containing data, materials, and analysis syntax of the study]. OSF. https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/7P3YA

  • Szabó, Z. P., Czibor, A., Neve, J., Restás, P., Hadarics, M., Szijjártó, L., Simon, E., Marót, J. L., Kun, A., & Bereczkei, T. (2023). Development of the Hungarian version of the Short Dark Triad questionnaire (SD3-HU): Psychometric properties and validity. Current Psychology, 42, 11628-11645. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-02470-4

  • Szabó, Z. P., Czibor, A., Restás, P., & Bereczkei, T. (2018). “The darkest of all”: The relationship between the Dark Triad traits and organizational citizenship behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 134, 352-356. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.04.026

  • Szabó, Z. P., Diller, S. J., Czibor, A., Restás, P., Jonas, E., & Frey, D. (2023). “One of these things is not like the others”: The associations between Dark Triad personality traits, work attitudes, and work-related motivation. Personality and Individual Differences, 205, Article 112098. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2023.112098

  • Szabó, Z. P., Kun, Á., Balogh, B. E., Simon, E., & Csike, T. (2022). Dark and strong?! The associations between dark personality traits, mental toughness and resilience in Hungarian student, employee, leader, and military samples. Personality and Individual Differences, 186(B), Article 111339. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2021.111339

  • Truhan, T. E., Wilson, P., Mõttus, R., & Papageorgiou, K. A. (2021). The many faces of dark personalities: An examination of the Dark Triad structure using psychometric network analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 171, Article 110502. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110502

  • van der Linden, D., Scholte, R. H., Cillessen, A. H. N., Te Nijenhuis, J., & Segers, E. (2010). Classroom ratings of likeability and popularity are related to the Big Five and the general factor of personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 44(5), 669-672. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2010.08.007

  • Wehner, C., & Ziegler, M. (2023). Narcissism and friendship quality: A longitudinal approach to long-term friendships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 40(2), 670-692. https://doi.org/10.1177/02654075221122023

  • Wolters, N., Knoors, H., Cillessen, A. H. N., & Verhoeven, L. (2014). Behavioral, personality, and communicative predictors of acceptance and popularity in early adolescence. Journal of Early Adolescence, 34(5), 585-605. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431613510403

  • Zeigler-Hill, V., Southard, A. C., & Besser, A. (2014). Resource control strategies and personality traits. Personality and Individual Differences, 66, 118-123. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.03.037

  • Zhang, F., You, Z., Fan, C., Gao, C., Cohen, R., Hsueh, Y., & Zhou, Z. (2014). Friendship quality, social preference, proximity prestige, and self-perceived social competence: Interactive influences on children’s loneliness. Journal of School Psychology, 52(5), 511-526. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2014.06.001

About the Authors

Zsolt Péter Szabó is an Associate Professor of Social Psychology at both Corvinus University Budapest and ELTE Eötvös Loránd University. His research primarily investigates the influence of dark personality traits on workplace dynamics and relational outcomes. Additionally, he has conducted extensive research on collective victimization and system justification within the context of Hungary.

Natália Zsuzsanna Orosz, a psychologist, graduated from ELTE Eötvös Loránd University. Her research primarily focuses on the relationships between personality traits and social acceptance.

Réka Gulyás, a psychologist, graduated from ELTE Eötvös Loránd University. Her research is related to workplace behavior of individuals with dark personality traits. Her studies primarily focus on exploring the relationships between dark personality traits and perfectionism.

András Láng is currently an Associate Professor of Clinical and Developmental Psychology at University of Pécs. His primary research interest lies in attachment theory. He is actively conducting research investigating the developmental and clinical aspects of Machiavellianism and the Dark Triad.